Through your skin to your heart and brain: A critical evaluation of physiological methods in Cognitive Translation and Interpreting Studies

Authors

  • Ana María Rojo Universidad de Murcia
  • Paweł Korpal Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań Department of Translation Studies

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.52034/lanstts.v19i0.533

Keywords:

Heart rate, heart rate variability, skin conductance, emotion, stress

Abstract

This article offers a critical appraisal of two experimental methods used to provide physiological measures of stress and emotions in translation and interpreting research, namely, the analysis of heart rate and heart rate variability, and skin conductance. This is a hands-on introduction to summarize information for fellow-researchers on what these methods are and what they tell us about our body and mind as well as to offer a comprehensive summary of practical applications and analysis standards. The first part of the article introduces the ways in which emotions are experienced and processed in the brain; it provides a framework for interpreting physiological arousal and its role in the perception and construction of emotions. The second part is structured in two parallel sections devoted to each of the two experimental methods. Both sections review existing research on these methods in Translation and Interpreting Studies and discuss the way in which each can best be used in experimental research. They offer suggestions on experiment planning, measurement, data analysis and data reporting. A final remark on ethics and triangulation is offered and some emerging challenges are addressed.

Author Biographies

Ana María Rojo, Universidad de Murcia

Ana María Rojo López is Professor of Translation at the University of Murcia (Spain), where she is currently Deputy Director of the International Doctoral School at the University of Murcia. Her current research interests focus mainly on the study of the translation process, with special emphasis on the role of emotions, creativity and other personality and individual differences. Her interests also lie within audience reception analysis and the contributions of Cognitive Linguistics to translation studies. She has authored and co-edited the following books and monograph issues: Contrastive Cognitive Linguistics (University of Murcia, 2003), Cognitive Linguistics: From Words to Discourse (University of Murcia, 2007), Step by Step. A Course in Contrastive Linguistics and Translation (Peter Lang, 2009), Trends in Cognitive Linguistics (Peter Lang, 2009), Diseños y métodos de investigación en traducción (Síntesis, 2013), Interdisciplinarity in Translation Studies (Peter Lang, 2016), Translation as an emotional phenomenon (John Benjamins, en prensa). She has also written many scholarly articles which have appeared either in specialised national and international journals or as book chapters published by several national and international publishing houses (Atrio, Anubar, Mouton de Gruyter, John Benjamins, Routledge, Sendebar, Babel, Languages in Contrast, Journal of Pragmatics, Meta, Across Languages and Cultures, Translation Spaces, Target, Multilingua, etc.). 

Paweł Korpal, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań Department of Translation Studies

Position: Assistant professor
URL: http://wa.amu.edu.pl/wa/korpal_pawel

References

AIIC (International Association of Conference Interpreters). (2002). Interpreter workload study: Full report. http://aiic.net/page/657/interpreter-workload-study-full-report/lang/1.

Allen, J. A., Armstrong, J. E., & Roddie, I. C. (1973). The regional distribution of sweating in man. Journal of Physiology, 235(3), 749–759. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010414

Barrett, L. F. (2017a). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx060.

Barrett, L. F. (2017b). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Houghton Mifflin Hardcourt.

Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 20–46. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2

Barry, R. J., Rushby, J. A., Wallace, M. J., Clarke, A. R., Johnstone, S. J., & Zlojutro I. (2005). Caffeine effects on resting-state arousal. Clinical Neurophysiology, 116(11), 2693–2700. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.008

Bassett, D. (2016). A literature review of heart rate variability in depressive and bipolar disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 50(6), 511–519. https://doi.org/10.1177/0004867415622689

Boucsein, W., Fowles, D. C., Grimnes, S., Ben-Shakhar, G., Roth, W. T., Dawson, M. E., & Filion, D. L. (2012). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1017–1034. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x

Bradley, M. M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (1990). Startle reflex modification: Emotion or attention? Psychophysiology, 27(5), 513–522. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb01966.x

Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

Braithwaite, J. J., Watson, D. G., Jones, R., & Rowe, M. (2013). A guide for analysing electrodermal activity (EDA) & skin conductance responses (SCRs) for psychological experiments. https://www.birmingham.ac.uk/Documents/college-les/psych/saal/guide-electrodermal-activity.pdf

Caldwell-Harris, C. L., & Ayçiçeği-Dinn, A. (2009). Emotion and lying in a non-native language. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71(3), 193–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.09.006

Cannon, W. B., & Rosenberg, C. E. (1932). Wisdom of the body. W.W. Norton and Company.

Cook, E. W., Hawk, L. W., Davis, T. L., & Stevenson, V. E. (1991). Affective individual differences and startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(1), 5–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.100.1.5

Chernigovskaya, T. V., Parina, I. S., Alekseeva, S. V., Konina, A. A., Urich, D. K., Mansurova, Yu. O., & Parin, S. B. (2019). Simultaneous interpreting: Characteristic of autonomic provision of extreme cognitive loads. Sovremennye Tehnologii v Medicine, 11(1), 132–140. https://doi.org/10.17691/stm2019.11.1.15

Christopoulos, G. I., Uy, M. A., & Yap, W. J. (2016). The body and the brain: Measuring skin conductance responses to understand the emotional experience. Organizational Research Methods, 22(1), 394–420. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428116681073

Davis, M. (1992). The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.), The amygdala: Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory and mental dysfunction (pp. 255–306). Wiley-Liss.

Doberenz, S., Roth, W. T., Wollburg, E., Maslowski, N. I., & Kim, S. (2011). Methodological considerations in ambulatory skin conductance monitoring. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80(2), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.002

Ekman P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411068

Frijda, N. H., & Scherer, K. R. (2009). Emotion definition (psychological perspectives). In D. Sander & K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Oxford companion to emotion and the affective sciences (pp. 142–143). Oxford University Press.

Furedy, J. J., Algan, O., Vincent, A., Demirgoren, S., & Pogun, S. (1999). Sexually dimorphic effect of an acute smoking manipulation on skin resistance but not on heart-rate during a cognitive verbal task. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 34, 219–226. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02688690

Gatti, E., Calzolari, E., Maggioni E., & Obrist, M. (2018). Emotional ratings and skin conductance response to visual, auditory and haptic stimuli. Scientific Data, 5, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1038/sdata.2018.120

Grabner, R. H., Brunner, C., Leeb, R., Neuper, C., & Pfurtscheller, G. (2007). Event-related EEG theta and alpha band oscillatory responses during language translation. Brain Research Bulletin, 72(1), 57–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.01.001

Halverson, S. L. (2010). Cognitive Translation Studies: Developments in theory and method. In G. Shreve & E. Angelone (Eds.), Translation and cognition (pp. 349–369). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/ata.xv.18hal

Harris, C. L. (2004). Bilingual speakers in the lab: Psychophysiological measures of emotional reactivity. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 25(2–3), 223–247. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434630408666530

Harris, C. L., Ayçiçeği, A., & Gleason, J. B. (2003). Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first than in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(4), 561–579. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000286

Heathers, J. A. J. (2014). Everything Hertz: Methodological issues in short-term frequency-domain HRV. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(177), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2014.00177

Hervais-Adelman, A., Moser-Mercer, B., & Golestani, N. (2015). Brain functional plasticity associated with the emergence of expertise in extreme language control. NeuroImage, 114, 264–274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.072

Hvelplund, K. T. (2014). Eye-tracking and the translation process: Reflections on the analysis and interpretation of eye-tracking data. Minding Translation, Special issue 1, 201–223. https://doi.org/10.6035/MonTI.2014.ne1.6

Iturregui-Gallardo, G., Serrano-Ratera, A., Méndez-Ulrich, J.-L., Soler-Vilageliu, O., & Jankowska, A. (2018, June 4–5). Testing the filmic experience: Audio subtitling and psychophysiology [Paper presentation]. Understanding Quality in Media Accessibility, Barcelona, Spain.

Jankowiak, K., & Korpal, P. (2018). On modality effects in bilingual emotional language processing: Evidence from galvanic skin response. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 47(3), 663–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9552-5

Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: The contribution of valence and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15(4), 241–251. https://doi.org/10.1515/REVNEURO.2004.15.4.241

Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(9), 3310–3315. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0306408101

Kim, H.-G., Cheon, E.-J., Bai, D.-S., Lee, Y. H., & Koo, B.-H. (2018). Stress and heart rate variability: A meta-analysis and review of the literature. Psychiatry Investigation, 15(13), 235–245. https://doi.org/10.30773/pi.2017.08.17

Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., Quigley, K. S., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrettt, L. F. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, Article 0069, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0069

Klonowicz, T. (1994). Putting one’s heart into simultaneous interpretation. In S. Lambert & B. Moser-Mercer (Eds.), Bridging the gap: Empirical research in simultaneous interpretation (pp. 213–224). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/btl.3.16klo

Korpal, P. (2016). Interpreting as a stressful activity: Physiological measures of stress in simultaneous interpreting. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 52(2), 297–316. https://doi.org/10.1515/psicl-2016-0011

Korpal, P. (2017). Linguistic and psychological indicators of stress in simultaneous interpreting. Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM.

Korpal, P., & Jankowiak, K. (2018). Physiological and self-report measures in emotion studies: Methodological considerations. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 49(4), 475–481.

Korpal, P., & Jasielska, A. (2019). Investigating interpreters’ empathy: Are emotions in simultaneous interpreting contagious? Target, 31(1), 2–24. https://doi.org/10.1075/target.17123.kor

Kurz, I. (2002). Physiological stress responses during media and conference interpreting. In G. Garzone & M. Viezzi (Eds.), Interpreting in the 21st century: Challenges and opportunities (pp. 195–202). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/btl.43.19kur

Kurz, I. (2003). Physiological stress during simultaneous interpreting: A comparison of experts and novices. The Interpreters’ Newsletter, 12, 51–67.

Laborde, S., Mosley, E., & Thayer, J. F. (2017). Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research: Recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00213

LeDoux, J. E. (1991). Emotion and the limbic system concept. Concepts in Neuroscience, 2, 169–199.

Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett. L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000446

Malik, M. (1996). Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. European Heart Journal, 17(3), 354–381. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.eurheartj.a014868

Melillo, P., Bracale, M., & Pecchia, L. (2011). Nonlinear heart rate variability features for real-life stress detection. Case study: Students under stress due to university examination. BioMedical Engineering OnLine, 10(96). https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-925X-10-96

Morimoto, T. (1978). Variations of sweating activity due to sex, age and race. Physiology and Pathophysiology of the Skin, 5, 1655–1666.

Muñoz Martín, R. (2010). On paradigms and cognitive translatology. In G. M. Shreve & E. Angelone (Eds.), Translation and cognition (pp. 169–187). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/ata.xv.10mun

Orero, P., Doherty, S., Kruger, J.-L., Matamala, A., Pedersen, J., Perego, E., Romero-Fresco, Rovira-Esteva, S., P., Soler-Vilageliu, O., & Szarkowska, A. (2018). Conducting experimental research in audiovisual translation (AVT): A position paper. Journal of Specialised Translation, 30, 105–126.

Pace-Schott, E. F., Amole, M. C., Aue, T., Balconi, M., Bylsma, L. M., Critchley, H., Demaree, H. A., Friedman, B. H., Kotynski Gooding, A. E., Gosseries, O., Jovanovic, T., Kirby, L. A. J., Kozlowska, K., Laureys, S., Lowe, L., Magee, K., Marin, M.-F., Merner, A. R., Robinson, J. L., Smith, R. C., Spangler, D. P., Van Overveld, M., & VanElzakker, M. B. (2019). Physiological feelings. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 103, 267–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.05.002

Penachini da Costa de Rezende Barbosa, M., Turri da Silva, N., Mícolis de Azevedo, F., Marcelo Pastre, C., & Marques Vanderlei, L. C. (2014). Comparison of Polar® RS800G3™ heart rate monitor with Polar® S810i™ and electrocardiogram to obtain the series of RR intervals and analysis of heart rate variability at rest. Clinical Physiology and Functional Imaging, 36(2), 112–117. https://doi.org/10.1111/cpf.12203

Piskorski, J., & Guzik, P. (2005). Filtering Poincaré plots. Computational Methods in Science and Technology, 11(1), 39–48. https://doi.org/10.12921/cmst.2005.11.01.39-48

PsychLab. (2003). Analysis for recordings in psychophysiology: Software manual. http://www.PsychLab.com/download.html.

Quintana, D. S. (2016). Statistical considerations for reporting and planning heart rate variability case-control studies. Psychophysiology, 54(3), 344–349. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12798

Ramos Caro, M. (2015). The emotional experience of films: Does Audio Description make a difference? The Translator, 21(1), 68–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/13556509.2014.994853

Ramos Caro, M. (2016). La traducción de los sentidos : Audiodescripción y emociones. LINCOM.

Rojo López, A. M., & Ramos Caro, M. (2014). The impact of translators’ ideology on the translation process: A reaction time experiment. In R. Muñoz Martín (Ed.), Minding Translation. MonTi, Special Issue 1, 247–272.

Rojo López, A. M., Ramos Caro, M., & Valenzuela Manzanares, J. (2014). The emotional impact of translation: A heart rate study. Journal of Pragmatics, 71, 31–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2014.07.006

Roziner, I., & Shlesinger, M. (2010). Much ado about something remote: Stress and performance in remote interpreting. Interpreting, 12(2), 214–247. https://doi.org/10.1075/intp.12.2.05roz

Scherer, K. R. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–317). Psychology Press.

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R. E., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (from Y1 – Y2). Consulting Psychologists Press.

Szpak, K. S., Alves, F., & Buchweitz, A. (in press). Perspective taking in translation: In search of neural correlates of representing and attributing mental states to others. In R. Muñoz Martín & S. L. Halverson (Eds.), Multilingual mediated communication and cognition (pp. 133–154). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429323867-6

Tourangeau, R., & Yan, T. (2007). Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 859–883. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.859

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

Waugh, C. E., Thompson, R. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2011). Flexible emotional responsiveness in trait resilience. Emotion, 11(5), 1059–1067. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021786

Downloads

Published

20-01-2021

How to Cite

Rojo, A. M., & Korpal, P. (2021). Through your skin to your heart and brain: A critical evaluation of physiological methods in Cognitive Translation and Interpreting Studies. Linguistica Antverpiensia, New Series – Themes in Translation Studies, 19. https://doi.org/10.52034/lanstts.v19i0.533